A RetroSearch Logo

Home - News ( United States | United Kingdom | Italy | Germany ) - Football scores

Search Query:

Showing content from https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s11292-011-9139-3 below:

Assessing the impact of imprisonment on recidivism

Abstract Objectives

There is debate about the extent to which imprisonment deters reoffending. Further, while there is a large literature on the effects of imprisonment, methodologically sound and rigorous studies are the exception due to problematic sample characteristics and study designs. This paper assesses the effect of imprisonment on reoffending relative to a prison diversion program, Community Control, for over 79,000 felons sentenced to state prison and 65,000 offenders sentenced to Community Control between 1994 and 2002 in Florida.

Methods

The effect of imprisonment on recidivism is examined within one-, two-, and three-year follow-up periods using Logistic Regression, Precision Matching, and Propensity Score Matching.

Results

Findings indicate that imprisonment exerts a criminogenic effect and that this substantive conclusion holds across all three methods.

Conclusions

The main contribution of this study is that various methods yield results that are at least in a similar direction and support overall conclusions of prior literature that imprisonment has a criminogenic effect on reoffending compared to non-incarcerative sanctions. Limitations and directions for future research are noted.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic

€34.99 /Month

Subscribe now Buy Now

Price includes VAT (Germany)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others Explore related subjectsDiscover the latest articles and news from researchers in related subjects, suggested using machine learning. Notes
  1. Defiance theory contends that the effect of an imposed punishment is contingent on how an offender perceives the punishment and the extent to which s/he is bonded to the punisher and society (Sherman 1993). However, defiance seems more apt to earlier stages of the punishment process, i.e., arrest, and thus is not described above with respect to the effect of incarceration on subsequent behavior.

  2. Other approaches that do not fit into the approaches used in the current study all share the characteristic that each attempts to construct some sort of counterfactual (Nagin et al. 2009:155–167). Specific examples may be found in Bhati and Piquero (2007), Manski and Nagin (1998), and Loughran et al. (2009).

  3. These authors are most concerned with exact matching on the variable age, because “offending rates are highly age dependent and because the postsanction outcome variable, offending rate or recidivism probability, necessarily must be measured over age.” They conclude that “it is very important in analyses of observational data to compare the postsanction offending rate of an imprisoned individual with that of one or more non-imprisoned individuals who are the same age” (p.138). Several studies match variables to one degree or another (i.e., some subset of variables in one study but not another), but our study does this in a very detailed manner (i.e., exact same age, etc.). Of course, exact matching cannot address the problem of unobserved imbalance among unobserved attributes as it only addresses observed imbalances among observed attributes.

  4. Community Control is an Intensive Supervision Program in which the offender is placed on “house arrest” which involves a home curfew established by the supervising officer. Also, contact standards are more extensive for this population relative to regular felony probation. The offender is restricted to his/her residence, with the exception of being allowed to work, attend treatment, visit the probation office, and limited other occasions that must be approved in advance by the community control officer.

  5. As is true of prior studies in this area, we do not account for the length of time served in our prison sample. This would require a different set of analyses which would incorporate incapacitation effects that is beyond the scope of this paper. We note that based on the prison sample derived from the precision matching described later, the prison group served an average of 22 months incarceration, the median was 18 months, and 84.6% served 36 months or less. This suggests that not accounting for prison exposure time may have a minimal impact on the findings.

  6. The primary offense is identified when multiple current convicted offenses are contained in the sentencing guidelines based on which one results in the highest number of total guidelines points.

  7. Nagin et al. (2009:184) argue that “it is particularly important to have extensive measurements of what the individual has “done” as measured by prior record and characteristics of the conviction offense. Because these factors represent the primary determinants of sentence type and length, it is vital that they be measured on as many dimensions as possible.” Our inclusion of the current offense quantified in nine different crime types and the seriousness of the current offense, based on the sentencing guidelines score and the use of three different indicators of prior record with no truncation of continuous measures, is our attempt at using the data available to us in order to meet their rigorous criteria and account for non-random assignment of sentences.

  8. An example of the lack of information relating to the precision matching process is the study of the effectiveness of Florida’s Community Control program versus prison in terms of recidivism conducted by Smith and Akers (1993). Their Table 1 (p.276) displays differences in summary statistics across the two punishment groups with sample sizes of 228 in the Community Control group and 266 in the prison group. The extent of the description of the matching process is captured in the statements, “in addition to the criminal comparability of two groups, as measured by the sentencing score, matching was also done on offense type in this study to increase even further the equivalency of the two groups. Offenders were also matched on age and previous violations of the two groups. However, offenders could not be matched on race or prior felonies and significant differences remained between the Community Control and prison groups on these two variables” (p.277). There are 228 Community Control cases and 266 in the prison group after matching. These different sample sizes intimate that more than one case in both groups was retained in instances in which the two groups matched on all the matching variables. This is further evidenced by the fact that the percentages across the two groups for categories of race and offense, which were matching variables, differed. If precision matching was conducted, as done in our study in which only one case from the prison and one case from Community Control samples were retained when there was more than one case in both groups that matched on all of the matching variables, the percentages across the groups would have been identical. However, this is not explicitly noted by the authors. In another study, Scarpitti and Stephenson (1968) matched youth who received probation (n = 943) to those who were placed in a non-residential group center (n = 100), a group center in which the youth resided (n = 67), and those placed in a state reformatory (n = 100). The groups differed significantly on race, school status and five characteristics of their families (Table 1). Three variables were used in the matching process; an index of family socioeconomic status, an index of delinquency history, and race. Only 44 boys matched across all four programs; however, descriptive statistics are not presented to indicate the level of equivalency of the two groups across the matching variables. We recognize that the lack of detail may have to do with publication requirements; nevertheless, we think that when matching and equivalency issues are involved more detail is best.

  9. In the analysis to follow using precision matching, it will be evident that the steps detailed above were replicated using different matching variables based on the inclusion of a varying number of covariates used in the matching process. An occurrence that appears on the surface to be counter-intuitive is that the number of cases in which precision matching generates will increase or decrease when more variables are added to the precision matching variable. Here, we provide an example to explain why this occurs. Let us assume we match the prison and Community Control datasets based on a matched variable, which includes five control variables, and the number of cases in the prison group that have a unique value on the matching variable is 10, and the number of cases with the same unique value on the matching variable in the Community Control group is 15. The final dataset of matched cases across the two sanctioning groups will be one after merging the two groups on unique values on the match variable and retaining one of these records. When a sixth variable is added to the match variable, by definition, the same number of cases will have the same value across the two groups based on the five matching variables, and one or more of the prison group cases can have the same matching variable value as one or more of the cases in the Community Control group. After retaining only one case from each unique value on the matching variable, the number of cases in the final dataset of matched prison and Community Control records will now be two. When a seventh or subsequent variables are added to the matching variable, the potential of adding additional cases to the final dataset continues, resulting in the final dataset with more cases than when fewer variables are used in the matching process. In contrast, the occurrence of a decrease in the number of matched cases as the number of matching variables increases occurs in the following circumstances. If there are no cases in the prison or Community Control samples that have the same value on an additional matching variable when increasing the number of matching variables by one, the cases that matched on the next lowest number of matching variables will no longer be included in the final matched dataset.

  10. Although somewhat counterintuitive, the reason why there are incremental increases in the number of cases in which precision matches were obtained from Matching #1 (n = 12,088) to Matching #4 (n  = 20,314) is because there are additional matching variables included. The reason for this occurrence is explained in endnote 9. The reason the number of cases decline from the Matching #4 model to the Matching #5 model is a result of the inclusion of the dichotomous variable of whether cases were recommended for a prison sentence under the guidelines (1) or a non-prison sanction (0). In contrast, the variables added in the models for Models #2, #3, and #4 are continuous variables which allow for more possible matches.

  11. Nagin et al. are emphatic about the importance of matching on age: “we regarded it as very important in analyses of observation data to compare the postsanction offending rate of an imprisoned individual with that of one or more non-imprisoned individuals who are the same age” (p.138). This assertion is grounded in the large number of studies that consistently find age as one of the best predictors of recidivism and that the judiciary makes punishment choices that result in different sanctioning populations that are substantively different in the relative age of offenders. In our study, the average age of those sentenced to prison is 34.5 versus 30.4 among those in Community Control, a substantive difference that requires serious attention in the analysis to achieve across group equivalency. To determine if there is empirical support for this assertion, we conducted precision matching analysis to assess if this emphasis on retaining age as a continuous measure was warranted. Specifically, and following Nagin et al.’s five-critical-variable recommendation, five different matching variables were created in which all included sex, race, Hispanic, current offense, and prior felonies, and each contained a different measure of age [a continuous measure in years, age as below or above the median (30 years), age in increments of 10 years (19 or younger, 20 to 29, etc.), and measures based on the percentage distribution into terciles, quartiles, and quintiles]. Comparisons in recidivism probabilities across the prison and Community Control groups created through precision matching using these five different approaches to quantifying age revealed evidence of a criminogenic effect of imprisonment regardless of how age was operationalized. However, the matching based on the continuous age measure produced the only significant effect and was substantively greater than the other four group comparisons using less precise measures of age. These results, which strongly support Nagin et al.’s concern over age, are available upon request.

  12. As more variables are added to the precision matching, the less criminogenic the samples become. This may indicate that precision matching may reduce the matched samples to a subpopulation of offenders who are higher risk Community Control offenders and lower risk prison offenders.

  13. Noteworthy is the fact that the number of cases which met the matching criteria using 15 variables (Match #2) increased incrementally from 6,044 in each sanctioning group to 10,157 when 17 of the 18 variables were utilized. This is counter to the concerns that some have raised when using precision matching to derive equivalent groups. Selltiz et al. (1959:105) state that the “more precise the matching, and the greater the number of factors on which matching is to take place, the greater the number of cases for which no match is available.” We speculate that this is due to the very large number of cases (144,416) which were available to derive the matching groups. Still, the inclusion of the final variable, whether prison was the recommended sentence, did result in a decrease in the number of cases from 10,157 in each group to 9,356. More generally, an anonymous reviewer observed that, as more matching variables are added in Table 6, the average seriousness of current and past criminal history decreases. The policy implications may be that the matched data only are conclusive for offenders with lower seriousness levels (current and past criminal history) and it is difficult to achieve matches (balance) for offenders in this population who get a prison sentence and have much more serious levels of criminality.

  14. A reviewer asked where, on a continuum of criminal risk based on the total sentencing guidelines points offenders who matched and did not match, fell. They raised the concern that precision matching may result in a subpopulation of offenders comprised of higher risk Community Control offenders and lower risk prison offenders. The Appendix Table presents the number and percentage of prison and community control cases within 17 ranges of the guidelines points and the number and percentage of community control cases that matched to a prison case based on the precision matching model #5 and those that did not match. The comparison was not possible to conduct based on the PSM analysis because the offender identification number and sentencing date were not retained in the resulting dataset and, therefore, it was not possible to merge the matched cases with the total sample using these two variables. The Appendix Table shows that, while a higher percentage of the community control cases are in the lower range of the total sentencing guidelines point distribution than prison cases, there are reasonable numbers and percentages of cases at the higher end of the distribution and that matched cases were retained at all levels of the total guidelines points. Additionally, while a higher percentage of the community control cases matched to prison case in the lower ranges of the guidelines points, which is expected given the higher number of cases in these groups, there is a similar percentage that matched as the point ranges increased to the maximum category of 100+. This indicates that the final matched sample is not overly dominated by higher risk Community Control cases and lower risk prison cases.

  15. Balance was not achieved using the entire sample of 144,416 cases. Several methods of transforming and truncating the distributions of the covariates were conducted which did not solve the imbalance problem. To investigate whether this was a sample size issue, we selected a random sample of 500 cases from each of the two groups and were able to attain balance. However, PSM models were generated using the entire sample and conclusions derived from these results are substantively the same as what is reported here from the random sample.

References

Download references

Author information Authors and Affiliations
  1. College of Criminology & Criminal Justice, Florida State University, 634 W. Call Street, Hecht House, Tallahassee, FL, 32306-1127, USA

    William D. Bales

  2. Program in Criminology, EPPS, University of Texas at Dallas, 800 W. Campbell Road, GR31, Richardson, TX, 75080, USA

    Alex R. Piquero

Authors
  1. William D. Bales
  2. Alex R. Piquero
Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alex R. Piquero.

Appendix Appendix Table 10 Number and percentage of community control cases matched to a prison case using Precision Matching #5 across categories of total sentencing guidelines points About this article Cite this article

Bales, W.D., Piquero, A.R. Assessing the impact of imprisonment on recidivism. J Exp Criminol 8, 71–101 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11292-011-9139-3

Download citation

Keywords

RetroSearch is an open source project built by @garambo | Open a GitHub Issue

Search and Browse the WWW like it's 1997 | Search results from DuckDuckGo

HTML: 3.2 | Encoding: UTF-8 | Version: 0.7.4